ebook img

Food Preference and Appetite after Switching between Sweet and Savoury Odours in Women PDF

16 Pages·2015·1.17 MB·English
by  
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Food Preference and Appetite after Switching between Sweet and Savoury Odours in Women

RESEARCHARTICLE Food Preference and Appetite after Switching between Sweet and Savoury Odours in Women MariëlleG.Ramaekers1,PieternelA.Luning1,CatrionaM.M.Lakemond1,MartinusA.J. S.vanBoekel1,GerritGort2,SanneBoesveldt3* 1 FoodQualityandDesign,WageningenUniversity,POBox17,6700AAWageningen,TheNetherlands, 2 Biometris,WageningenUniversityandResearchCentre,POBox100,6700ACWageningen,The Netherlands,3 DivisionofHumanNutrition,WageningenUniversity,POBox8129,6700EVWageningen, TheNetherlands *[email protected] a11111 Abstract Background Exposuretofoododoursincreasestheappetiteforcongruentfoodsanddecreasesthe appetiteforincongruentfoods.However,theeffectofexposuretoavarietyoffoododours, OPENACCESS asoftenoccursindailylife,isunknown. Citation:RamaekersMG,LuningPA,Lakemond CMM,vanBoekelMAJS,GortG,BoesveldtS(2016) Objective FoodPreferenceandAppetiteafterSwitching Investigatehowswitchingbetweensweetandsavouryodoursaffectstheappetiteforsweet betweenSweetandSavouryOdoursinWomen. PLoSONE11(1):e0146652.doi:10.1371/journal. andsavouryproducts. pone.0146652 Editor:JohnI.Glendinning,BarnardCollege, Design ColumbiaUniversity,UNITEDSTATES Thirtywomen(age:18-45y;BMI:18.5-25kg/m2)intenselysmelledthecontentsofcupsfilled Received:September7,2014 withbanana,meatorwater(no-odour)inawithin-subjectdesignwithfourcombinations: Accepted:December21,2015 no-odour/banana,no-odour/meat,meat/bananaandbanana/meat.Participantsreceived Published:January11,2016 onecombinationpertestday.Ineachcombination,twocupswithdifferentfillingswere smelledforfiveminutesaftereachother.Treatmentorderwasbalancedasmuchaspossi- Copyright:©2016Ramaekersetal.Thisisanopen accessarticledistributedunderthetermsofthe ble.Theeffectsofpreviousexposureandcurrentodourontheappetitefor(in)congruent CreativeCommonsAttributionLicense,whichpermits sweetandsavouryproducts,andodourpleasantnesswereanalysed.Achangefrommeat unrestricteduse,distribution,andreproductioninany tobananaodourorbananatomeatodourwasreferredtoasswitch,whereasachange medium,providedtheoriginalauthorandsourceare fromno-odourtomeatodourorno-odourtobananaodourwasno-switch. credited. DataAvailabilityStatement:Allrelevantdataare Results withinthepaperanditsSupportingInformationfiles. Thecurrentodour(P<0.001),asopposedtothepreviousexposure(P=0.71),determined Funding:Theauthorshavenosupportorfundingto report. theappetitefor(in)congruentsweetandsavouryproducts,alreadyoneminuteaftera switchbetweensweetandsavouryodours.Thepleasantnessoftheodourdecreaseddur- CompetingInterests:Theauthorshavedeclared thatnocompetinginterestsexist. ingodourexposure(P=0.005). PLOSONE|DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0146652 January11,2016 1/16 FoodPreferenceandAppetiteafterSwitchingOdours Conclusions Afteraswitch,theappetiteforspecificproductsquicklyadjustedtothenewodourandfol- lowedthetypicalpatternasfoundduringodourexposureinpreviousstudies.Interestingly, theappetiteforthesmelledfoodremainedelevatedduringodourexposure,knownassen- sory-specificappetite,whereasthepleasantnessoftheodourdecreasedovertime,previ- ouslytermedolfactorysensory-specificsatiety.Thisseemingcontradictionmayresultfrom differentmechanismsunderlyingtheodour-inducedanticipationoffoodintakeversusthe decreaseinhedonicvalueduringprolongedsensorystimulation. Introduction Unhealthyeatinghabitssuchasunhealthyfoodchoicesorovereatingincreasetheprevalence ofobesity,diabetes,cancer,cardiovascularandotherdiseases[1–3].Therefore,itisimportant tounderstandhowvariousfactors,forexamplesensoryprocesses,influenceoureatingbehav- iour.Sensoryprocessesplayaroleinfoodselectioninseveralways.First,associationsbetween thenutrientcompositionandthesensorypropertiesoffoods,suchasappearance,smelland taste,areformedduetorepeatedexposureinourdailylives[4].Theseassociationspartly determinethepleasurethatisderivedfromfoods[5,6],whereuponpleasantnessinfluences foodselection.Moreover,theseassociationsalsofacilitatetheestimationofthenutrientcom- positionoffoodsbasedonthesensoryproperties[4]andthisinformationcanbeusedforfood selection,forexampleincaseofnutrientdeficits[7,8].Furthermore,recentlyconsumedfoods modulatefoodpreference,whichislikelydrivenbytheneedforvariety.Forexample,thepref- erenceforsavouryproductsdecreasesaftereatingasavourymeal,aphenomenonreferredto assensory-specificsatiety[9,10].Finally,externalfactorssuchasexposuretosight,tasteor smelloffoodschangeourfoodpreference[11–15].Ithasbeenwidelydemonstratedthatexpo- suretofoodcuesincreasesthepreferenceforthecuedfood[14–20].Forexample,sweetodours increasedtheappetiteforsweetproductsandsavouryodourstheappetiteforsavouryproducts [17,18].Moreover,sweetodoursalsodecreasedtheappetiteforsavouryproductsandsavoury odoursforsweetproducts[17,18]. Theprecisemechanismbehindthesefindingshasnotyetbeenelucidated,althoughwehave proposedthatthisincreaseinappetiteforcongruentfoodsanddecreaseinappetiteforincon- gruentfoodsmaybecausedbycephalicphaseresponses[17].Cephalicphaseresponsespre- parethebodyfortheintakeanddigestionoffoods[21–23]andareelicitedbyfoododoursand otherfoodcues.Ingeneral,sweetnessisassociatedwithsugarcontentandsavourinesswith proteincontent[4,7],withdistinctroutesofdigestionfordifferentmacronutrients.Therefore, determiningthetypeoffoodbyexposuretofoodcuesbeforeingestion,maypreparethe bodyforthedigestionofthespecificmacronutrientsoftheanticipatedfoods[17,24].It maybethatoncethebodyispreparedfortheintakeofafoodwithacertain(macro) nutrientcomposition,itislessfavourabletoingestafoodwithaverydifferent(macro) nutrientcomposition[17]. Indailylifethough,forexamplebystrollingthrougha(super)market,exposuretoavariety offoodcuesthatprimeforawidevarietyoffoods,mayinduceconfusioninthebody.Previous exposurestofoodcuesmaypossiblyinterferewithexposurestonewfoodcues.Ifourbody indeedspecificallypreparesfortheintakeofcuedfoods,thenitmaytakesometimetoswitch theappetitefor(in)congruentfoodsaccordingtothecharacteristicsofnewfoodcues. PLOSONE|DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0146652 January11,2016 2/16 FoodPreferenceandAppetiteafterSwitchingOdours Toourknowledge,theeffectofswitchingbetweendifferentfoodcuesongeneralappetite andfoodpreferencehasnotbeeninvestigatedbefore.Theobjectiveofthecurrentstudywasto determinehowswitchingbetweensweetandsavouryfoododoursaffectstheappetiteforsweet andsavouryproducts,foodpreferenceandgeneralappetite.Generalappetiteandtheappetite forsweetandsavouryproductsweremeasuredatseveraltimepointsduringodourexposureto exploreifpossiblechangesafterswitchingareimmediateortaketime.Theresultscouldpro- videinsightintheprocessesbehindtheeffectoffoodcuesonfoodpreferenceinreallife. MaterialsandMethods Participants Thirtyhealthywomenaged18–45(21.6±4.7)yandBMI18.5–25(21.9±1.3)kg/m2were recruitedfromWageningenandsurroundingsbetween7and14February2013,byusingane- maillistwithpotentialparticipantsthatwasset-upbytheDivisionofHumanNutrition, WageningenUniversity(Fig1).Exclusioncriteriawere:dislikeforbanana,bananapie(Dutch pastry),steamedmeatorbeefsoup(score<5onanine-pointscale),smoking,pregnancyor Fig1.Flowchartthatshowstheparticipantenrolmentinthisstudy. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0146652.g001 PLOSONE|DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0146652 January11,2016 3/16 FoodPreferenceandAppetiteafterSwitchingOdours breastfeedingduringthelastsixmonths,lackofappetite,followinganenergy-restricteddiet orchangeinbodyweight>5kgduringthelast2months,hypersensitivitytoanyofthefoods understudyorbeingavegetarian.Itwasexplainedtotheparticipantsthattheinfluenceofsen- sorysignalsonfoodchoicewasinvestigated.Afterthestudy,theparticipantswereinformed aboutthefullstudyobjectives.Twenty-nineparticipantscompletedthestudyandonepartici- pantmissedthelastsession(no-odour/meat)duetoillness.Allparticipantssignedaninformed consentformbeforeparticipation.AllprocedureswereinaccordancewiththeHelsinkiDecla- rationof1975(asrevisedin1983).ThisstudywasexemptfromapprovalbytheMedicalEthi- calCommitteeofWageningenUniversity,becausetherulesofconductinthepresentresearch werenotinterventionist,withverylittleburdentotheparticipants.Thisstudywasregisteredat theDutchtrialregister(NTR3830;www.trialregister.nl/trialreg/admin/rctview.asp?TC=3830). Theparticipantsreceivedacompensationof25euro. Experimentaldesign Theparticipantsintenselysmelledthecontentsofcupsfilledwithbanana,meatorwater(no- odour)inawithin-subjectdesignwithfourcombinations:no-odour/banana,no-odour/meat, meat/bananaandbanana/meat(Fig2).Eachparticipantwasexposedtoonecombinationwith twosuccessiveodourexposuresandaone-minutebreakinbetweenexposures,oneachtest day.Thesefourcombinationsweresetuptocomparetheappetiteforspecificproductsaftera switchwiththeappetiteforspecificproductsafterno-switch(no-odourascontrol).Forexam- ple,tocomparetheappetiteforbanana,duringsmellingbananaaftersmellingno-odour(no- switch),withtheappetiteforbanana,duringsmellingbananaaftersmellingmeat(switch).We usedno-odour/bananaandno-odour/meatascontrolsinsteadofbanana/bananaandmeat/ meat,toexcludeundesireddifferencesinadaptationorspecificsatiationduetodifferencesin exposuretimetoaspecificodour.Eachodourwassmelledintenselyforfiveminutes.Appetite measurementsweretakenat1and5minutesduringexposuretothefirstodourandat7and 11minutesduringexposuretothesecondodour.Theorderofthecombinationswasbalanced overtheparticipants,andasmuchaspossibleoverthetestdays.Eachparticipantwassched- uledonfourseparatedays,preferablyonceperweekatthesametimeofthedaybetween11.20 hand13.40h.Theeffectsofthepreviousexposureandthecurrentodourontheappetitesfor (in)congruentsweetandsavouryproducts,foodpreference,generalappetiteandodourpleas- antnesswereanalysed.Achangefrommeattobananaodourorfrombananatomeatodour wasreferredtoasswitchbetweenodours,whereasachangefromno-odourtomeatodouror no-odourtobananaodourwasno-switch(Fig2). Odours Theparticipantssmelledthecontentsofcupsthatcontainedeither10gofwater(no-odour),a tablespoonofmediumripemashedbanana(bananaodour)oratablespoonofwarmsteamed meat(stoofvlees,Coertjens,Belgium;meatodour).Atissueandaplasticlidwereplacedover thefillingtopreventvisualcuesandodourcontaminationinthesensoryroom. Standardizinghungerstate Thevisitsofeachparticipantwerescheduledatthesametimeofthedaytostandardizethe individualhungerstate.Onthefirsttestday,participantswereinstructedtoconsumeanormal amountofbreakfast,atleast2.5hoursbeforethestartoftheexperiment.Afterthistime,only waterandweakteawereallowed.Ontheremainingtestdays,theparticipantswererequested toconsumethesamebreakfastasonthefirsttestday.Thediariesinwhichparticipants PLOSONE|DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0146652 January11,2016 4/16 FoodPreferenceandAppetiteafterSwitchingOdours Fig2.Schematicrepresentationoftheexperimentaldesignincludingexplanationoftheterminologyusedinthedataanalysisandresultsections. *Currentodourispresentlysmelledbytheparticipants,whichcanbeeitherthefirstorthesecondodourinacombination.**Previousexposureisthe exposurethatprecededthecurrentodour.***Codeisusedintheresultssectionanddefinesthecombinationofpreviousexposureandcurrentodour.**** Exposuregroupisusedforthedata-analysis. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0146652.g002 recordedtheirbreakfast,changesinphysicalactivityandhealthproblemswerecheckedfor possibleconfoundersandtoincreasecommitmenttothestudyrules. Procedure Uponthefirstarrival,theparticipantswereinformedontheprocedure,includingademon- strationofhowtosmellintenselybyplacingthenoseabovethecupandbreathingthroughthe nose.Theparticipantsthensettleddowninsensoryboothswithoverpressure(Biotechnionof WageningenUniversity)withtwocupsinfrontofthem.Instructionsweregivenonacom- puterscreen(E-prime,v2.0).Participantsfirstfilledouttheappetitequestionnaireatbaseline (t=0).Subsequently,theywererequestedtoremovetheplasticlidfromthefirstcup,while keepingthetissueonthecup,andthenintenselysmellthecontentofthefirstcup.Theappetite PLOSONE|DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0146652 January11,2016 5/16 FoodPreferenceandAppetiteafterSwitchingOdours questionnairewasrepeated1and5minutesafterthestartofthesmelling.Theparticipants wereencouragedbythetextonthecomputertocontinueintensesmellingduringthewhole fiveminutesofodourexposure.Afterfiveminutes,theparticipantsplacedbackthelidonthe firstcupandhadaone-minutebreak.Subsequently,theyfollowedthesameprocedurewith thesecondcupandfilledouttheappetitequestionnaireatt=7and11minutes.Afterfivemin- utesofintenselysmellingthecontentofthesecondcup,participantsplacedbackthelidand filledoutthefoodpreferencequestionnaire.Attheendofthesession,theparticipantshadto choosebetweenabananaandabreadrollwithsteamedmeatthatwereplacedonaplatein frontofthem.Theparticipantswereaskedabouttheirthoughtsonthestudyobjectiveatthe endofthestudyinanendevaluation. Measurements Anappetitequestionnaireandfoodpreferencequestionnairewerefilledoutduringthe experiment. Theappetitequestionnairemeasuredhungeranddesire-to-eatovertimeon100mmcom- puterizedvisualanaloguescales(VAS,notatall–very)[25].Besides‘general’appetite,the appetitefor15individualproductswasmeasuredon100mmVASinarandomisedorder(for example,‘Howlargeisyourappetiteforabananaatthismoment?;notatall–very)[25].These productsweredividedintobanana,meat,sweet,savouryandstapleproducts.Bananaand bananapiewereselectedasbananaproductsandbreadrollwithsteamedmeatandbeefsoup asmeatproducts.Sweetproductsweremango,sweetpastry‘tompouce’,strawberryyoghurt andM&M’s.Savouryproductswerebreadrollwithegg,tomatosoup,cheeseandsaltedpea- nuts.Stapleproductswerebreadbun,croissantandpancake.Inaddition,odourintensity(100 mmVAS,notatall–very)andfeelingwell(100mmVAS,notatall–very)wereaddedtothe appetitequestionnairetocheckiftheodoursbecameoverwhelmingoraffectedthepartici- pants’well-being.Finally,odourpleasantnesswasmonitoredovertime(100mmVAS,notat all–very). Thefoodpreferencequestionnaire(FPQ)wasacomputerizedtaskaspreviouslyusedin Ramaekersetal.[17]andbasedonworkofFinlaysonetal.[26,27](E-prime,v2.0;Psychology softwaretools,Sharpsburg,PA,USA)measuringfoodpreferenceattheendofthe10minutes ofexposuretotheodours.Oneachtrial,twofoodsweresimultaneouslyshownonacomputer screenusingdigitalcolourphotographs.Theparticipantswereaskedtochoosethefoodthat theywouldliketoeatmostatthatmoment.Thefoodsonthephotographswerethesameas thefoodsintheappetitequestionnaire,plusadditionallybananasweets,andlittlesnacksau- sages.Allbananaproductswerecomparedagainstallnon-bananaproducts,includingmeat products.Inaddition,allmeatproductswerecomparedagainstallnon-meatproducts,leading to84comparisons.Thefrequencyofeachchosenproductwasdetermined. Dataanalysis Allvariableswereanalysedwithmixedlinearmodelstoaccountforthecorrelationsbetween therepeatedmeasures(SASversion9.1.3;SASInstituteInc.,Cary,NC),wherethefixedpartof themodelcapturesthetreatmentstructureandtherandompartthecorrelationsamongobser- vations[28].AlldegreesoffreedomwerecalculatedaccordingtothemethodbyKenwardand Roger[29].P-values<0.05(two-sided)wereconsideredsignificant. Allfiguresshowmeanvalues±SDoftherawdata.Resultsinthetextareestimated means±SE,usingamixedmodel.Resultsinthetextontransformeddatawereback-trans- formedtotheoriginalscaletofacilitateinterpretation. PLOSONE|DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0146652 January11,2016 6/16 FoodPreferenceandAppetiteafterSwitchingOdours Appetitequestionnaire. Participantsreceivedfourcombinationsofodours,butthedata fromtheappetitequestionnairewerenotcomparedbetweencombinations.Instead,westudied theeffectsofpreviousexposure(no-smelling,no-switch,switch),currentodour(no-odour, banana,meat),exposuretime(exposuretimetothecurrentodour,either1or5min;Fig2) andtheirinteractionsonthefollowingvariables:generalappetite,odourpleasantness,odour intensity,feelingwellandchangeinappetiteforspecificproducts. Wedefinedafactor‘exposuregroup’asacombinationofpreviousexposure,currentodour andtime(1,5,7,11min),becausenotallpossible3x3x4combinationsexisted.‘Exposure group’representsthe14actualcombinations(Fig2).Wecomparedgroupmeansof‘exposure group’byusingcontraststoanalysetheeffectsofpreviousexposure(exposuregroups3+4vs7 +8+9+10vs11+12+13+14),currentodour(1+2vs3+4+9+10+13+14vs5+6+7+8+11+12), exposuretimetothecurrentodour(1vs5minand7vs11min;exposuregroups1+3+5+7+9 +11+13vs2+4+6+8+10+12+14),productandtheirinteractions.Toinvestigatewhetherprevi- ousexposurehadaneffect,B,nBandmBwerecomparedagainsteachother(exposuregroups 5+6vs7+8vs11+12)andM,nMandbMwerecomparedagainsteachother(exposuregroups 3+4vs9+10vs13+14;seeFig2forthecodes).Incasetheeffectofpreviousexposurewasnot significant,thedatafromB,nBandmBweregroupedasbananaodour(exposuregroups5+6 +7+8+11+12),M,nMandbMasmeatodour(exposuregroups3+4+9+10+13+14)andNas no-odour(exposuregroups1+2).Whentherewasasignificanteffectofpreviousexposure, onlytheratingsattime1and5minwereusedforanalysisoftheeffectofcurrentodour(expo- suregroups1+2vs3+4vs5+6). Generalappetite,odourpleasantness,odourintensityandfeelingwell:Beforestatistical analysis,generalappetitewascalculatedastheaverageofthehungerandthedesire-to-eat scores.Subsequently,ratingsforgeneralappetite,odourpleasantness,odourintensityandfeel- ingwellwerelogittransformed,usingln((y/100+0.01)/(1-y/100+0.01))tostabilizethevari- ance.Thefixedpartofthemixedmodelconsistedofthefactor‘exposuregroup’.Furthermore, generalappetiteattime=0wasusedascovariateforgeneralappetite.Therandompartofthe mixedmodelsconsistedofrandomeffectsforsessionsandparticipants.Forgeneralappetite, weusedanautoregressiveorder-1correlationmatrixforthecorrelationsamongrepeatedmea- surementsat1,5,7and11min.Forvariablesodourpleasantness,odourintensityandfeeling wellacompoundsymmetrycorrelationmatrixwasused.Correlationsandresidualvariances wereallowedtodifferbetweencurrentodoursforodourpleasantnessandodourintensity. Changeinappetiteforspecificproducts:Beforestatisticalanalysis,themeanchangein appetiteforbananaproductswascalculatedbyaveragingthechangescores(currentratings minusratingsattime=0)oftheratingsoftheappetiteforbananaandbananapie.Similarly, theratingsoftheappetiteformeat,sweet,savouryandstapleproductswereaveragedsepa- ratelyovertherespectiveproducts.Thefixedpartofthemixedmodelconsistedofthefactor ‘exposuregroup’(Fig2),anextrafactor‘product’(banana,meat,sweet,savoury,staple)and theinteractionof‘product’with‘exposuregroup’.Therandompartofthemixedmodelcon- sistedofrandomeffectsforsessionsandparticipantsandanautoregressiveorder-1correlation matrixforthecorrelationsamongrepeatedmeasurementsat1,5,7and11min.Additionally, anunstructuredcovariancematrixwasaddedtoallowforunequal(co)variancesamongprod- uctgroupsperpersonandtime.Correlationsandresidualvarianceswereallowedtodiffer betweencurrentodours. Foodpreferencequestionnaire(FPQ). Theeffectofcombination(no-odour/banana,no- odour/meat,meat/bananaandbanana/meat)onfoodpreferencewasinvestigatedusingthe FPQ.Thefoodpreferencedataweretransformedusingarcsine(sqrt(frequency/max)),with ‘max’representingthemaximumnumberoftimesaproductcouldbechoseninaset,tostabi- lizevariances.AllcomparisonsbetweenproductsintheFPQweresplitintosevensets,with PLOSONE|DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0146652 January11,2016 7/16 FoodPreferenceandAppetiteafterSwitchingOdours eachsetcontainingcomparisonsoftwotypesofproducts.Forexample,the‘banana-staple’set containsthecomparisons:bananavsbreadbun,bananavscroissant,bananavspancake, bananapievsbreadbun,bananapievscroissant,bananapievspancake,bananasweetsvs breadbun,bananasweetsvscroissant,bananasweetsvspancake.Fixedeffectfactorswere combination,setandtheirinteraction.Anunstructuredcovariancematrixspecifiedthe(co) variancesbetweensets.Perset,wewereinterestedincomparisonsbetweencombinations. Results Appetiteforbanana,meat,sweet,savouryandstapleproducts Fig3showstheaveragechangesintheappetitesforbanana,meat,sweet,savouryandstaple products,furtheronnamedastheappetiteforspecificproducts.Previousexposureandits interactionswithexposuretime,currentodourorproductdidnotsignificantlyaffecttheappe- titeforspecificproducts(allP>0.05;Table1).Therefore,allratingsweregroupedpercurrent odour(banana,meatandno-odour).Theinteractionbetweencurrentodourandproductwas significant(P<0.001):thechangesintheappetitesforbanana,meat,sweet,savouryandstaple productsdidnotdifferamongeachotherintheno-odourcombination(P=0.13),butdiffered duringexposuretobanana(P<0.001)andmeat(P<0.001)odour.Exposuretobananaodour increasedtheappetiteforbananaproducts(P<0.001),decreasedtheappetiteformeat (P=0.026)andsavoury(P=0.028)productsandhadnosignificanteffectonsweet(P=0.10) andstaple(P=0.48)products,comparedwithno-odour.Exposuretomeatodourincreased theappetiteformeat(P<0.001)products,decreasedtheappetiteforbanana(P<0.001)and sweet(P<0.001)productsandhadnoeffectonsavoury(P=0.46)andstapleproducts (P=0.72),comparedwithno-odour. Preferenceforbanana,meat,sweet,savouryandstapleproducts, measuredwithFPQ Thepreferenceof84pairsoffoodpictureswasassessed,comparingbananaandmeatproducts againsteachotherandagainstsavoury,sweetandstapleproducts(Table2).Whensetsof bananaandmeatproductswereoffered,thebananaproductswerechosenmoreoftenwhen thelastsmelledodourwasbanana,thanwhenthelastsmelledodourwasmeat(allP<0.05).In thebanana-savouryandbanana-staplesets,thebananaproductsweremoreoftenchosenafter theno-odour/bananacombination,thanaftercombinationsno-odour/meatandbanana/meat (allP<0.05).Inthemeat-sweetset,themeatproductswerechosenlessoftenaftercombination no-odour/banana,thanaftercombinationsno-odour/meatandbanana/meat(allP<0.05).The assessmentofthe84pairstook3to4minpersession. Actualfoodchoice Aftertheodourexposure,thirteenparticipantsalwayschoseabananaandnineparticipants alwayschoseabreadbunwithsteamedmeat.Eightoutof30participantsswitchedtheirfood choicebetweensessions.Thechoiceforbananavsbreadrollsteamedmeatwasasfollows:19 vs11inno-odour/banana,19vs11inmeat/banana,12vs17inno-odour/meatand17vs13in banana/meat. Generalappetite Generalappetiteratingswereonaverage79mm.Therewerenosignificantdifferencesingen- eralappetitebetweenthefourcombinationsattime=0min(F =1.8;P=0.16).General 3,83.4 appetiteratingsattimes1,5,7and11werestronglyrelatedtotheappetiteratingsattime=0 PLOSONE|DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0146652 January11,2016 8/16 FoodPreferenceandAppetiteafterSwitchingOdours Fig3.Meanchangeinappetiteforbanana,meat,staple,sweetandsavouryproductsovertime. Scoresduringexposureto(A)no-odour/banana,(B)no-odour/meat,(C)meat/bananaand(D)banana/meat, measuredon100mmVAS.ThenumbersbetweenthebracketsrepresenttheSDatrespectively1,5,7and 11minutes.Thedashedlinerepresentstheswitchbetweenodours. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0146652.g003 PLOSONE|DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0146652 January11,2016 9/16 FoodPreferenceandAppetiteafterSwitchingOdours Table1. F-valueswithdegreesoffreedomandP-valuesofallfactorswithfixedeffectsforthechange inappetiteforspecificproducts. Changeinappetiteforproducts Fixedeffects Exposuretime F =0.6 P=0.55 2,336 Previousexposure F =0.8 P=0.54 4,281 Currentodour F =4.0 P=0.019 2,303 Product F =9.6 P<0.001 4,254 ExpTime*Previous F =1.2 P=0.32 4,298 ExpTime*Current F =2.6 P=0.073 2,279 ExpTime*Product F =1.1 P=0.38 8,473 Previous*Current F =0.7 P=0.50 2,258 Previous*Product F =0.80 P=0.71 16,502 Current*Product F =16 P<0.001 8,445 All3-or4-wayinteractions P>0.20 doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0146652.t001 (F =250;P<0.001;Table3).Generalappetitedidnotchangesignificantlyovertime 1,124 (P=0.52).Theeffectofpreviousexposureongeneralappetitewasborderlinesignificant (P=0.08)andtherefore,theeffectofodourwasinvestigatedonlyattimes1and5min.There werenosignificantdifferencesbetweenmeat,bananaandno-odourattimes1and5min(Mvs BvsN;P=0.25).Theinteractionbetweenpreviousexposureandcurrentodourwasnotsignif- icant(F =1.2;P=0.31). 2,167 Odourpleasantness Fig4showsthepleasantnessofthecurrentlysmelledodour.Therewasasignificantinteraction betweencurrentodourandexposuretime(F =3.5;P=0.033;Table3).Ratingsdecreased 2,152 onaverageby4mmfrom1minto5minexposuretimeduringexposuretomeatandbanana odour(P=0.005).Noeffectofexposuretimewasfoundduringexposuretono-odour (P=0.61).Previousexposurehadnosignificanteffectonratedodourpleasantness(F = 4,68.4 0.3;P=0.88).Thepleasantnessratingsofmeatandbananaodourswerehigherthanofno- Table2. Meanpercentageoftimesaproductwaschosenpersetofproducts,afterexposuretodifferentcombinations,measuredwiththefood preferencequestionnaire. Set Product1 Banana- Banana- Banana- Banana- Meat- Meat- Meat- Product2 Meat* Savoury* Sweet* Staple* Savoury# Sweet# Staple# Nrofcomparisonsperset 18 12 12 9 12 12 9 No-odour/banana 62a 60a 43 48a 46 33a 33 Meat/banana 58a 50ab 42 40ab 47 41ab 33 No-odour/meat 42b 47b 38 33b 53 56b 44 Banana/meat 42b 44b 43 33b 54 54b 46 SuperscriptwithdifferentlettersdenotesignificantdifferencesatP<0.05 *Meanpercentageoftimesthatabananaproductwaschosen #Meanpercentageoftimesthatameatproductwaschosen doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0146652.t002 PLOSONE|DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0146652 January11,2016 10/16

Description:
Design. Thirty women (age: 18-45y; BMI: 18.5-25kg/m2) intensely smelled the contents of cups filled with banana, meat or water (no-odour) in a within-subject design with four combinations: no-odour/banana, no-odour/meat, meat/banana and banana/meat. Participants received one combination per test
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.