ebook img

Complete abolition of reading and writing ability with a third ventricle colloid cyst PDF

25 Pages·2017·1.12 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Complete abolition of reading and writing ability with a third ventricle colloid cyst

Complete abolition of reading and writing ability with a third ventricle colloid cyst: implications for surgical intervention and proposed neural substrates of visual recognition and visual imaging ability. BARKER, Lynne <http://orcid.org/0000-0002-5526-4148>, MORTON, Nicholas, ROMANOWSKI, Charles A J and GOSDEN, Kevin Available from Sheffield Hallam University Research Archive (SHURA) at: http://shura.shu.ac.uk/8639/ This document is the author deposited version. You are advised to consult the publisher's version if you wish to cite from it. Published version BARKER, Lynne, MORTON, Nicholas, ROMANOWSKI, Charles A J and GOSDEN, Kevin (2013). Complete abolition of reading and writing ability with a third ventricle colloid cyst: implications for surgical intervention and proposed neural substrates of visual recognition and visual imaging ability. BMJ case reports, 2013. Copyright and re-use policy See http://shura.shu.ac.uk/information.html Sheffield Hallam University Research Archive http://shura.shu.ac.uk Complete abolition of reading and writing ability with third ventricle colloid cyst: Implications for surgical intervention and the proposed neural substrates of visual recognition and visual imaging ability. Journal: BMJ Case Reports Manuscript ID: bcr-2013-200854.R1 Manuscript Type: Unusual presentation of more common disease/injury Date Submitted by the Author: n/a Complete List of Authors: Barker, Lynne; Sheffield Hallam University, Psychology Morton, Nicholas; Rotherham, Doncaster and South Humber Mental Health NHS Foundation Trust, Neurorehabilitation services Romanowski, Charles; Sheffield Teaching Hospital NHS Foundation Trust, Dept; of Academic Radiology Gosden, Kevin; Rotherham, Doncaster and South Humber Mental Health NHS Foundation Trust, Neurorehabilitation services Neurosurgery 620 < Surgery 1349, Rehabilitation medicine 1348, Accidents, injuries 861 < Occupational and environmental medicine 842, Keywords: Memory Disorders < Neurology 200, Coma and raised intracranial pressure 205 < Neurology 200 Page 1 of 23 Submission template for full cases • All case reports MUST be submitted online using this Word template http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/bmjcasereports o You will be asked for more detailed information on submission where you can also upload images, multimedia files, etc o Further details are available in the Instructions for authors http://casereports.bmj.com/site/about/guidelines.xhtml • You must have signed informed consent from patients (or relatives/guardians) before submitting to BMJ Case Reports. Please anonymise the patient’s details as much as possible, eg, specific ages, occupations. Consent forms are available in several languages http://group.bmj.com/products/journals/patient-consent-forms/consentforms • You or your institution must be a Fellow of BMJ Case Reports in order to submit. Fellows can submit as many cases as they like, access all the published material, and re-use any published material for personal use and teaching without further permission. o For more information on rates and how to purchase your fellowship visit http://casereports.bmj.com/site/about/becomeafellow.xhtml o Contact your librarian or head of department to see if your institution already has a Fellowship TITLE OF CASE Do not include “a case report” Complete abolition of reading and writing ability with a third ventricle colloid cyst: Implications for surgical intervention and proposed neural substrates of visual recognition and visual imaging ability. SUMMARY Up to 150 words summarising the case presentation and outcome (this will be freely available online) We report a rare case (DD) unable to read (alexic) and write (agraphic) after mild head injury. He had preserved speech and comprehension, could spell aloud, identify words spelled aloud and copy letter features. He was unable to visualize letters but showed no problems with digits. Neuropsychological testing revealed general visual memory, processing speed and imaging deficits. Imaging data revealed a 8mm colloid cyst of the third ventricle that splayed the fornix. Little is known about functions mediated by fornical connectivity but this region is thought to contribute to memory recall. Other regions thought to mediate letter recognition and letter imagery, Visual Word Form Area and visual pathways, were intact. We remediated reading and writing by multi-modal letter retraining. The study raises issues about the neural substrates of Revised June 2011 Page 1 of 15 Page 2 of 23 reading, role of fornical tracts to selective memory in absence of other pathology, and effective remediation strategies for selective functional deficits. BACKGROUND Why you think this case is important – why did you write it up? The current case is rare in the presentation of global incapacity to read and write with no language disruption or evidence of gross pathology. We are not aware of other reported cases presenting with global alexia and agraphia. Patients typically present with partial alexia/agraphia and show substantial lesions to occipito-parietal regions and/or visual pathways (cf Déjèrine1). In addition, most reported cases of colloid cysts are post-surgery because the cyst has often grown relatively large by the time the patient presents to medical services increasing intracranial pressure, compressing neural structures and requiring surgical intervention. Hence, almost all cases reported in the literature to our knowledge show secondary effects on neural structures beyond displacement of fornices due to cyst size and location or as a result of surgery, or a combination of both. These may include: Fornix transection, callosal tract disconnection, frontal atrophy, mamillary body atrophy, ventricular enlargement, hippocampal atrophy and evidence of gliosis 2,3. Thus, it is difficult to extrapolate the precise function of fornical tracts and surrounding structures from earlier post-surgical documented cases, and there are scant reported data revealing how a cyst abutting the fornix in an otherwise healthy brain, might disrupt cognition. The present case demonstrates severe memory dysfunction possibly as a consequence of white (fornical) matter tract disruption. Arguably, the nature of the deficit underlying memory, reading and writing deficits is visual (visual letter recognition and visual imagery ability). Findings challenge assumptions that a specific Visual Word Form Area in the brain governs reading and writing ability. Thus the case is medically rare, exceptional in the functional/clinical presentation and challenges current hypotheses regarding the neural substrates of reading/writing ability. CASE PRESENTATION Presenting features, medical/social/family history DD was aged 18 when he sustained a minor TBI (Traumatic Brain Injury) and neck injury in a road traffic accident (RTA). There was a post-traumatic amnesia of at least one hour. Accident and emergency records show that DD presented with neck injury and no loss of consciousness, Revised June 2011 Page 2 of 15 Page 3 of 23 although there was a period of confusion lasting up to 5 days following the injury where DD reported loss of episodic recall for events. He was diagnosed with whiplash injuries. Imaging data showed a colloid cyst to the third ventricle abutting the fornix (see Figure 2 and 3). DD reported that he had become progressively illiterate over a period of two months post- accident; he first became aware of it when letters on TV appeared alien ‘like foreign symbols.’ On referral he complained of inability to read or write, (although he was able to draw), memory deficits, headaches, irritation and excessive levels of fatigue on exertion. There were no clinical signs of receptive or expressive dysphasia, word finding or comprehension deficits based on screening subtests from the Test for Reception of Grammar (TROG4) and Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language Processing in Aphasia (PALPA5); social use of language appeared normal. He presented with profound inability to recognize, image or write letters based on free recall. DD was unable to recognise or write any letter (with the exception of O and X which he recognised as mathematical symbols) or words on initial interview. When words were spelled aloud he could correctly identify the word. He was unable to sign his name or write his address but could spell his name and address verbally. When asked to read a registration plate he could only read the numbers. He performed arithmetic sums presented visually. Some difficulties were noted with the spelling of exception words (sieve, dove, for example), and correctly identifying irregular words spelled aloud on a letter-by-letter basis (‘sow’ interpreted as ‘stitch’ i.e. ‘sew’). He repeated letters aloud phonetically and commented that he could not ‘see’ the letter or word ‘in his mind’s eye.’ He could recite the alphabet slowly forwards and backwards. INVESTIGATIONS If relevant DD completed a battery of measures of intelligence, memory, executive function and mood state to establish his functional profile of deficit and sparing. Test results are presented in Table one. [Insert table 1 here] Interpretation of test performance: General Intelligence (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III 6). Revised June 2011 Page 3 of 15 Page 4 of 23 General intellectual functioning scores fell within the average range for Full-Scale, Performance and Verbal IQ indices. Conversion analyses of Verbal and Performance subtest scaled scores showed that Digit Span (SS = 6) a verbal subtest, and Digit-Symbol Coding a performance subtest scores (SS = 5) were statistically significantly lower (p .05) than the mean score for Verbal (m = 11.8) and Performance (m = 9) subtests overall. Although the Digit Span subtest is verbally mediated and Digit-symbol is performance based they both require manipulation of abstract symbols, which may explain DD’s significantly poorer ability on these measures compared to other subtests. Vocabulary (SS = 17), Picture Completion (SS = 15) and Matrix Reasoning (SS = 13) scaled scores were statistically higher than overall Verbal and Performance means indicating excellent knowledge of the meaning of words, holistic visual processing, and problem solving with abstract visual symbols that do not have a visual structure similar to letters. The score for Verbal Comprehension Index fell within the high average range, Perceptual Organization index was Borderline, with Working Memory and Processing Speed Index scores ranging from extremely low to impaired. Verbal (110) and Performance IQ (99) scores were significantly different (p = .05) with DD showing poorer ability on performance compared to verbal tasks. Verbal Comprehension Index (120) score was significantly higher (p = .05) than Perceptual Organization Index (109), Working Memory Index (95), and Processing Speed Index scores (75). Processing Speed Index score was significantly lower than Working Memory Index score, and both were significantly lower than other Index scores (p = .05). His core profile indicated general working memory, information processing speed and perceptual organization weaknesses compared to verbal ability. Visuospatial perception: Visual Object and Space Perception Battery (VOSP 7). DD showed intact visual perceptual and visuospatial abilities on subtests indicating that low-level perceptuo-spatial deficits were not driving poor scores on other measures. Visual and auditory attention: the Test of Everyday Attention (TEA8) DD showed impaired selective visual attention in the first minute of the task. Performance improved in the second minute falling within the borderline impaired range. He performed within Revised June 2011 Page 4 of 15 Page 5 of 23 normal ranges on auditory attentional subtests (elevator counting task). Attention and visual and verbal memory: The Attention and Memory Information Processing Battery (AMIPB 9). DD performed within the low average range for immediate verbal recall for the Story Recall subtest of episodic memory, and at borderline to impaired range for immediate recall for the Figure Recall subtest. He showed impaired delayed recall for verbal and visual information, and an impaired level of retention over time for both verbal and visual material on Story Recall and Figure Recall subtests (Figure 1). [Insert Figures 1 a, b, and c here] Figure recall drawings revealed poor retention of visual information from immediate copy with picture present to immediate and delayed copies without target stimulus present for figural features of image indicating severe disruption to visual memory. Verbal and non-verbal visual recognition and recall: The Doors and People test 10. DD performed at the impaired range on the Doors test, a non-verbal visual recognition task and the People test, a task requiring acquisition of the name of four face stimuli across three trials. He performed at ceiling on the Shapes test requiring four simple shapes to be copied and reproduced after a delay of several seconds contrasting with impaired Figure Recall performance shown on the AMIPB9. To summarize, performance on intelligence and memory measures revealed impaired information processing speed often seen after head injury. DD also showed impaired working memory, poor perceptual organization and performance-based IQ compared to verbal ability, impaired visual attention, impaired retention of complex verbal and visual material over time, impaired immediate retention of visual material, impaired ability to recognize visual stimuli amid distracters after a brief delay, impaired ability to match visual stimuli (faces) with corresponding verbal labels and intact ability to copy and remember simple visual shapes. Scores on the Hospital Revised June 2011 Page 5 of 15 Page 6 of 23 Anxiety and Depression scale (HADS11) reflected a mild degree of anxiety and depressive symptoms in the preceding week. Imaging data Images were obtained on a 3Tesla Philips Achieva scanner (Philips Medical Systems, Best, The Netherlands). The sequences obtained were; axial and coronal TSE T2 (TR 2067, TE 80); axial and coronal proton density (TR 2067, TE 17.78); T1 volume (TR 10.18, TE 4.691, flip angle 8, 0.8mm isotropic voxel size). The skull base demonstrated some congenital asymmetry with underdevelopment of the right petrous temporal bone. As a result of this there was some asymmetry to the shape of the overlying temporal lobes. Brain regions were otherwise normal. There was no evidence of any visible contusional damage to the lingual (medial occipitotemporal) gyrus or the fusiform (lateral occipitotemporal) gyrus on either side. Other areas commonly involved by contusional damage were also normal. Gradient echo and susceptibility weighted imaging are very sensitive to the presence of haemosiderin, a lasting product of intraparenchymal haemorrhage. There was no evidence of any haemosiderin staining in any region of the brain. The only abnormality was the presence of a small (8mm maximal diameter) colloid cyst at a typical location at the anterior end of the third ventricle. This was not obstructing the foramina of Monro and there was no lateral ventricular dilatation; however the cyst splayed the columns of the fornix. [Insert figure 2 here] [Insert figure 3 here] DIFFERENTIAL DIAGNOSIS If relevant TREATMENT If relevant Therapy intervention approaches were designed as follows; rote rehearsal method required DD to study each randomly selected target letter by concentrating on the shape of the letter and any Revised June 2011 Page 6 of 15 Page 7 of 23 distinctive figural features. The speech therapist presented the letter and enunciated the sound of the letter three times for approximately one second per letter to reinforce learning. DD completed both types of training within each session in counterbalanced order over ten sessions conducted over a three-week period. For the multimodal procedure DD was instructed to study the letter in detail and to think of an alternative stimulus beginning with the letter that he associated with the letter shape, for example ‘m’ reminded him of mountains because of up and down strokes, ‘s’ reminded him of a snake because of its curved shape, and ‘e’ reminded him of an eye. This strategy created a phonemic link between the letter and the personal mnemonic prompt to enhance relevance and optimise learning. We also devised verbal descriptions of letter features, for example ‘c’ described as an incomplete circle with a section missing on the right hand side, ‘l’ described as a long vertical stick. The letters were reproduced in enlarged format. We asked DD to trace the shape of the letter while the therapist described visual features of the letter and reminded DD of the personal mnemonic prompt he had generated for the letter. On the third presentation DD was asked to generate a word beginning with the chosen letter (different from the mnemonic word generated earlier to aid recognition) to strengthen semantic word-letter associations. In this way multi-sensory (visual, auditory and tactile/kinetic) associations were made for each letter. We anticipated that this might prove more effective than the rote rehearsal method because that relies predominantly on visual/phonemic integration alone whilst the multimodal method synthesised multi-sensory information to enhance learning and depended upon mnemonic cues specifically generated by, and personal too, DD. [Insert figure 4 here] Figure four shows accuracy of ability to select target letters to a verbal prompt, and recognise and name target letters across ten therapy sessions in the rote rehearsal intervention. The letter ‘i’ was recognised as familiar and named correctly after one second following ten treatment sessions for the rote rehearsal method. Although four letters of this set (w, p, q and i) were considered to be familiar to DD, ‘n’, ‘v’ and ‘w’, and ‘p’ and ‘q’ were regularly confused. The average time to name each letter on session ten was 5 seconds, although only one response was accurate. Revised June 2011 Page 7 of 15 Page 8 of 23 [Insert figure 5 here] Figure five shows accuracy scores for letter selection familiarity and naming across ten sessions using the multimodal procedure. Plotted data show that the complete letter set was identified, named and rated as familiar by DD following the ten treatment sessions. Mean response time for letter naming was five seconds per letter, the same as for the rote rehearsal set, possibly indicating that although the multimodal letters had been learned identification was not automatic as with fluent reading. However, results of the initial intervention suggested that a multimodal approach to letter learning was more effective than rote learning in facilitating re-learning of letters and this approach was used for the 12 letters of the alphabet not yet studied in the second treatment block. OUTCOME AND FOLLOW-UP Results graphed in figure six show that the 12 remaining letters of the alphabet were remediated after ten additional therapeutic sessions using the multimodal intervention for letter relearning. [Insert figure 6 here] DISCUSSION Include a very brief review of similar published cases Complete inability to recognize a single letter of the alphabet is rarely seen in alexia cases and when it does occur ability to write usually remains intact1. Abstract representation of visual letters/words has been termed the ‘visual word form’12-13. The assumption is that recognition of the ‘word form’ enables fluent reading to occur across font, case, colour and size12-13. The term agraphia denotes individuals who have lost the capacity to write with reading ability generally intact; such cases are rare although more frequently documented than combined alexia and agraphia in the same individual as reported here14. Miozzo and Caramazza15 reported the case of GV, an 84 year-old woman who sustained a large left posterior brain lesion. GV was impaired at reading letters and numbers, and naming pictures, objects and colours. However, performance accuracy for naming tactile stimuli was at ceiling indicating intact access to non-visual word information. Agraphia indicates impaired writing when limb movements are intact14. This type of Revised June 2011 Page 8 of 15

Description:
colloid cyst: implications for surgical intervention and proposed neural substrates of remediation strategies for selective functional deficits. in the presentation of global incapacity to read and write with no language . the letter three times for approximately one second per letter to reinforc
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.