ebook img

C George Boeree, PhD: Personality Theory: A Biosocial Approach, © 2009. 1 PDF

224 Pages·2010·1.39 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview C George Boeree, PhD: Personality Theory: A Biosocial Approach, © 2009. 1

C George Boeree, PhD: Personality Theory: A Biosocial Approach, © 2009. 1 Welcome! I believe that we are getting to the point where we might be ready for a unified theory of personality. This online book is my attempt to look at the study of personality in terms of issues rather than individual (and often contradictory) theories. Those of you who have read some of my other online work will recognize many bits and pieces of it, but I have reorganized the material in a more thematic way so that, while you will see some of the historically significant theories and research, you will also see more clearly how these are leading to something more integrated. There are also many places where I have added my own ideas regarding where I think the field is headed. The goal here is not to convince you of one theory or another, but rather to help you sort through the work of others and formulate your own best understanding of what it means to be a person. I hope that you find this book interesting and helpful! This is dedicated to the spirit of Gardner Murphy, whose book Personality: A Biosocial Approach was my introduction to psychology. - George Boeree C George Boeree, PhD: Personality Theory: A Biosocial Approach, © 2009. 2 Table of Contents 1. Introduction 8.4 Helping 1.1 Science 8.5 Aggression 1.2 Methods 8.6 Society 1.3 Phenomenology 1.4 Beware! 9. Temperaments 9.1 Ancient Greeks 2. Consciousness 9.2 Carl Jung 2.1 Qualities 9.3 Hans Eysenck 2.2 The senses 9.4 Baby temperaments 2.3 Neediness 9.5 The big five 2.4 The I and the me 9.6 Intelligence 2.5 Perspectives 9.7 Maleness and femaleness 9.8 Autism 3. Perception 9.9 Antisociality 3.1 The "raw material" of perception Conduct disorder 3.2 Gestalts Borderline personality disorder 3.3 The perception of utility Antisocial personality disorder 9.10 Conclusions 4. Interaction 4.1 Anticipation 10. Society 4.2 Imagination 10.1 Constructed realities 4.3 Thinking 10.2 Culture 4.4 Person perception 10.3 Getting a picture of a culture 4.5 Social interaction 10.4 Cultural typologies 4.6 The phenomenal field 10.5 The band 10.6 The tribe 5. Adaptation 10.6 Civilization 5.1 Learning 10.7 The age of the individual 5.2 Remembering and forgetting 5.3 Constructs 11. The social unconscious 5.4 Mental structures 11.1 Conformity 5.5 Inferences 11.2 Norms An example: Attraction 6. Emotions Another example: Personal space 6.1 The surprise family One more example: Time 6.2 The fear family 11.3 Roles 6.3 The anger family 11.4 Status 6.4 The sadness family 11.5 Society and our mental health 6.5 The eagerness family 6.6 The happiness family 12 Self-determination 6.7 The boredom family 12.1 Gordon Allport 12.2 Alfred Adler 7. Motivation 12.3 Existentialism 7.1 Curiosity and play 12.4 Free-will vs determinism 7.2 Social motivation 7.3 Habit 13. Values 7.4 The hierarchy and the daimonic 13.1 Alfred Adler 7.5 Actualization 13.2 Gordon Allport 13.3 Abraham Maslow 8. Instinct 13.4 Carl Rogers 8.1 Animal instincts 13.5 Existential psychology 8.2 Sex 13.6 Viktor Frankl 8.3 Children 13.6 Values and anticipation 13.7 Transcendence of self C George Boeree, PhD: Personality Theory: A Biosocial Approach, © 2009. 3 Table of Contents 14. Stages Authoritarianism 14.1 Sigmund Freud Other forms of neurosis 14.2 Erik Erikson Psychosis 14.3 Jean Piaget The peach 14.4 Family 14.5 The infant 17. Defenses and coping strategies The sensorimotor stage 17.1 Sigmund Freud Erikson's first stage 17.2 Alfred Adler 14.6 The young child 17.3 Karen Horney The toddler 17.4 Erich Fromm The preschooler 17.5 Balance theory Preoperational stage 17.6 Cognitive dissonance theory 14.7 The older child 17.7 Carl Rogers Concrete operations 14.8 Adolescence 18. Specific disorders Peers 18.1 Anxiety disorders Ego identity Panic attacks and panic disorder Formal operations Agoraphobia 14.9 Adulthood Specific phobias Middle age Social phobia 14.10 Late Adulthood Generalized anxiety disorder Strokes Post-traumatic stress disorder Alzheimer's 18.2 Obsessive-compulsive disorder Similar disorders 15 Moral development 18.3 Mood disorders 15.1 Kohlberg's theory Bipolar disorders 15.2 Bronfenbrenner's theory 18.4 Trance disorders 15.3 Seven perspectives Somatoform disorders The autistic perspective Dissociative disorders The authoritarian perspective 18.5 Psychosis The rationalist perspective Schizophrenia The mechanistic perspective Cultural variation The cybernetic perspective 18.6 Alienation The phenomenological perspective Causes The transcendental perspective Consequences Development of perspective Back to the beginning 16. Psychological disorders 19. Therapy 16.1 Sigmund Freud 19.1 Sigmund Freud 16.2 Carl Jung 19.2 Carl Jung 16.3 Alfred Adler 19.3 Otto Rank 16.4 Karen Horney 19.4 Alfred Adler 16.5 Carl Rogers 19.5 Carl Rogers 16.6 Existential psychology 19.6 George Kelly 16.7 Viktor Frankl 19.7 Albert Ellis 16.8 Albert Ellis 19.8 Existential therapy 16.9 Buddhist psychology 19.9 Viktor Frankl 16.10 A biosocial approach 19.10 Rollo May Developmental aspects 19.11 The eightfold path Fear of nothing 19.12 Biosocial therapy 20. Conclusion Bibliography C George Boeree, PhD: Personality Theory: A Biosocial Approach, © 2009. 4 1. Introduction Personality psychology is the study of the person, that is, the whole human individual. Most people, when they think of personality, are actually thinking of personality differences – types and traits and the like. This is certainly an important part of personality psychology, since one of the outstanding characteristics of persons is that they can differ from each other quite a bit. But the main part of personality psychology addresses the broader question: "What is it to be a person?" Personality psychologists view their field of study as being at the top (of course) of a pyramid of other fields in psychology, each more detailed and precise than the ones above. Practically speaking, that means that personality psychologists must take into consideration biology (especially neurology), evolution and genetics, sensation and perception, motivation and emotion, learning and memory, culture and society, developmental psychology, psychopathology, psychotherapy, and whatever else might fall between the cracks. Since this is quite an undertaking, personality psychology may also be seen as the least scientific (and most philosophical) field in psychology. It is for this reason that most personality courses in colleges still teach the field in terms of theories. We have dozens and dozens of theories, each emphasizing different aspects of personhood, using different methods, sometimes agreeing with other theories, sometimes disagreeing. Like all psychologists – and all scientists – personality psychologists yearn for a unified theory, one we can all agree on, one that is firmly rooted in solid scientific evidence. Unfortunately, that is easier said then done. People are very hard to study. We are looking at an enormously complicated organism (one with "mind," whatever that is), embedded in not only a physical environment, but in a social one made up of more of these enormously complicated organisms. Too much is going on for us to easily simplify the situation without making it totally meaningless by doing so! 1.1 Science It is a mistake to believe that a science consists in nothing but conclusively proved propositions, and it is unjust to demand that it should. It is a demand only made by those who feel a craving for authority in some form and a need to replace the religious catechism by something else, even if it be a scientific one. Science in its catechism has but few apodictic precepts; it consists mainly of statements which it has developed to varying degrees of probability. The capacity to be content with these approximations to certainty and the ability to carry on constructive work despite the lack of final confirmation are actually a mark of the scientific habit of mind. – Sigmund Freud The traditional, idealized picture of science looks like this: Let’s start with a theory about how the world works. From this theory we deduce, using our best logic, a hypothesis, a guess, regarding what we will find in the world of our senses, moving from the general to the specific. This is rationalism. Then, when we observe what happens in the world of our senses, we take that information and inductively support or alter our theory, moving from the specific to the general. This is empiricism. And then we start again around the circle. So science combines empiricism and rationalism into a cycle of progressive knowledge. Now notice some of the problems science runs into: If my theory is true then my hypothesis will be supported by observation and/or experiment. But notice: If my hypothesis is supported that does not mean that my theory is true. It just means that my theory is not necessarily wrong! On the other hand, if my hypothesis is not supported, that does in fact mean that my theory is wrong (assuming everything else is right and proper). So, in science, we never have a theory we can say is unequivocally true. We only have theories that have stood the test of time. They haven’t been shown to be false... yet! C George Boeree, PhD: Personality Theory: A Biosocial Approach, © 2009. 5 1. Introduction This is one of the things that most people don’t seem to understand about science. For example, people who prefer creationism over evolution will say that, since evolution is "only a theory," then creationism is just as legitimate. But evolution has been tested again and again and again, and the observations scientists have made since Darwin have held up tremendously well. It's like saying that a thoroughbred race horse is "just a horse," and therefore any old nag is just as good! On the other hand, creationism fails quickly and easily. Carbon dating shows that the world is far older than creationists suggest. There are fossils of species that no longer exist. There is a notable lack of fossils of human beings during the dinosaur age. There are intermediate fossils that show connections between species. There are examples of species changing right before our eyes. There is a vast body of related knowledge concerning genetics. But with every piece of evidence shown to the creationists, they respond with what the logicians call an ad hoc argument. An ad hoc argument is one that is created after the fact, in an attempt to deal with an unforeseen problem, instead of being a part of the theory from the beginning. So, if there is a rock that is too old, or a fossil that shouldn’t be, the creationist might respond with "well, God put that there in order to test our faith," or "the days in Genesis were actually millions of years long" or "mysterious are the ways of the Lord." Obviously, creationism is based on faith, not science. Science is always a work in progress. No one believes in evolution, or the theory of relativity, or the laws of thermodynamics, the same way that someone believes in God, angels, or the Bible. Rather, we accept evolution (etc.) as the best explanation available for now, the one that has the best reasoning working for it, the one that fits best with the evidence we have. Science is not a matter of faith. Science is, of course, embedded in society and influenced by culture and, like any human endeavor, it can be warped by greed and pride and simple incompetence. Scientists may be corrupt, scientific organizations may be dominated by some special interest group or another, experimental results may be falsified, studies may be poorly constructed, scientific results may be used to support bad policy decisions, and on and on. But science is really just this method of gaining knowledge – not knowledge we can necessarily be certain about, but knowledge that we can rely upon and use with some confidence. For all the negatives, it has been the most successful method we have tried. 1.2 Methods If you take two different forms of measurement – such as a measuring tape and a weight scale – and we measure the height and weight of a few hundred of our nearest and dearest friends, we can examine whether the two measures relate to each other somehow. This is called correlation. And, as you might expect, people's heights and weights do tend to correlate: The taller you are, generally speaking the heavier you are. Of course, there will be some folks who are tall but quite light and some who are short but quite heavy, and lots of variation in between, but there will indeed be a modest, but significant, correlation. You might be able to do the same thing with something involving personality. For example, you might want to see if people who are shy are also more intelligent than people who are outgoing. So develop a way to measure shyness-outgoingness and a way to measure intelligence (an IQ test!), and measure a few thousand people. Compare the measures and see if they correlate. In the case of this example, you would likely find little correlation, despite our stereotypes. Correlation is a popular technique in psychology, including personality. C George Boeree, PhD: Personality Theory: A Biosocial Approach, © 2009. 6 1. Introduction What correlation can't help you with is finding what causes what. Does height somehow cause weight? Or is it the other way around? Does being shy cause you to be smarter, or does being smarter cause you to be shy? You can't say. It could be one way or the other, or in fact there could be some other variable that is the cause of both. That's where experimentation comes in. Experiments are the "gold standard" of science, and all of us personality psychologists wish we had an easier time doing them. In the prototypical experiment, we actually manipulate one of the variables (the independent one) and then measure a second variable (the dependent one). So, for example, you can measure the degree of rotation of the volume knob on your radio, and then measure the actually volume of the music that comes out of the speakers. What you would find, obviously, is that the further you turn the knob, the louder the volume. They correlate, but this time, because the knob was actually manipulated (literally in this case) and the volume measured after, you know that the rotation of the knob is in some way a cause of the volume. Taking this idea into the world of personality, we could show people scary movies that have been rated as to how scary they are. Then we could measure their anxiety (with an instrument that measures how sweaty our hands get, for example, or with a simple test where we ask them to rate how frightened they are). Then we can see if they correlate. And, of course, they would to some degree. Plus we now know that the scarier the movie, the more scared we get. A breakthrough in psychological science! There are several things that make measurement, correlation, and experiments difficult for personality psychologists. First, it isn't always easy to measure the kinds of things we are interested in in any meaningful way. Even the examples of shyness-easygoingness and intelligence and anxiety are iffy at best. How well do people recognize their own anxiety? How well does a sweat-test relate to anxiety? Can a paper- and-pencil test really tell you if you are smart or shy? When we get to some of the most important ideas in personality – ideas like consciousness, anger, love, motivations, neurosis – the problem looks at present to be insurmountable. Another difficulty is the problem of control. In experiments, especially, you need to control all the irrelevant variables in order to see whether the independent variable actually affects the dependent variable. But there are millions of variables impacting us at every moment. Even our whole history as a person is right there, influencing the outcome. No sterile lab will ever control those! Even if you could control many of the variables – the psychological version of a sterile lab – could you now generalize beyond that situation? People act differently in a lab than at home. They act differently when they are being observed than when they do in private. Experiments are actually social situations, and they are different from other social situations. Realism might be the answer, but how does one accomplish realism at the same time as one keeps control? Then there's the problem of samples. If a chemist works with a certain rock, he or she can be pretty confident that other samples of the same rock will respond similarly to any chemicals applied. Even a biologist observing a rat can feel pretty comfortable that this rat is similar to most rats (although that has been debated!). This is certainly not true for people. In psychology, we often use college freshmen as subjects for our research. They are convenient – easily available, easy to coax into participation (with promises of "points"), passive, docile.... But whatever results you get with college freshmen, can you generalize them to people in factories? to people on the other side of the world? to people 100 years ago or 100 years in the future? Can you even generalize to college seniors? This problem transcends the issues for quantitative methods to qualitative methods as well. C George Boeree, PhD: Personality Theory: A Biosocial Approach, © 2009. 7 1. Introduction What about qualitative methods, then? Qualitative methods basically involve careful observation of people, followed by careful description, followed by careful analysis. The problem with qualitative methods is clear: How can we be certain that the researcher is indeed being careful? Or, indeed, that the researcher is even being honest? Only by replicating the studies. There are as many qualitative methods as there are quantitative methods. In some, the researcher actually introspects – looks into his own experiences – for evidence. This sounds weak, but in fact it is ultimately the only way for a researcher to directly access the kinds of things that go on in the privacy of his or her own mind! This method is common among existential psychologists. Other researchers observe people "in the wild," sort of like ethologists watch birds or chimps or lions, and describe their behavior. The good thing here is that it is certainly easier to replicate observations than introspections. Anthropologists typically rely on this method, as do many sociologists. One of the most common qualitative method in personality is the interview. We ask questions, sometimes prearranged ones, sometimes by the seat of our pants, of a variety of people who have had a certain experience (such as being abducted by a UFO) or fall into a certain category (such as being diagnosed as having schizophrenia). The case study is a version of this that focusses on gaining a rather complete understanding of a single individual, and is the basis for a great deal of personality theory. 1.3 Phenomenology Anyone who wants to know the human psyche will learn next to nothing from experimental psychology. He would be better advised to abandon exact science, put away his scholar's gown, bid farewell to his study, and wander with human heart through the world. There in the horrors of prisons, lunatic asylums and hospitals, in drab suburban pubs, in brothels and gambling-hells, in the salons of the elegant, the Stock Exchanges, socialist meetings, churches, revivalist gatherings and ecstatic sects, through love and hate, through the experience of passion in every form in his own body, he would reap richer stores of knowledge than text-books a foot thick could give him, and he will know how to doctor the sick with a real knowledge of the human soul. – Carl Jung Phenomenology is the careful and complete study of phenomena, and is basically the invention of the philosopher Edmund Husserl. Phenomena are the contents of consciousness, the things, qualities, relationships, events, thoughts, images, memories, fantasies, feelings, acts, and so on, which we experience. Phenomenology is an attempt to allow these experiences to speak to us, to reveal themselves to us, so we might describe them in as unbiased a fashion as possible. If you've been studying experimental psychology, this might seem like another way of talking about objectivity. In experimental psychology, as in science generally, we try to get rid of our nasty subjectivity and see things as they truly are. But the phenomenologist would suggest that you can't get rid of subjectivity, no matter how hard you try. The very attempt to be scientific means approaching things from a certain viewpoint – the scientific viewpoint. You can't get rid of subjectivity because it isn't something separate from objectivity at all. Most of modern philosophy, including the philosophy of science, is dualistic. This means that it separates the world into two parts, the objective part, usually conceived of as material, and the subjective part, consciousness. Our experiences are then the interaction of this objective and subjective part. Modern science has added to this by emphasizing the objective, material part, and de-emphasizing the subjective part. Some call consciousness an "epiphenomenon," meaning an unimportant by-product of brain chemistry and other material processes, something that is, at best, a nuisance. Others, such as B. F. Skinner, see consciousness as nothing at all. C George Boeree, PhD: Personality Theory: A Biosocial Approach, © 2009. 8 1. Introduction Phenomenologists suggest that this is a mistake. Everything the scientist deals with comes "through" consciousness. Everything we experience is colored by "the subjective." But a better way to put it is that there is no experience that does not involve both something which is experienced, and something which is experiencing. This idea is called intentionality. So phenomenology asks us to let whatever we are studying – whether it be a thing out there, or a feeling or thought inside us, or another person, or human existence itself – to reveal itself to us. We can do this by being open to the experience, by not denying what is there because it doesn't fit our philosophy or psychological theory or religious beliefs. It especially asks us to bracket or put aside the question of the objective reality of an experience – what it "really" is. Although what we study is always likely to be more than what we experience, it is not something other than what we experience. Phenomenology is also an interpersonal undertaking. While experimental psychology may use a group of subjects so that the subjectivity can be removed from their experiences statistically, phenomenology may use a group of co-researchers so that their perspectives can be added together to form a fuller, richer understanding of the phenomenon. This is called intersubjectivity. This method, and adaptations of this method, have been used to study different emotions, psychopathologies, things like separation, loneliness, and solidarity, the artistic experience, the religious experience, silence and speech, perception and behavior, and so on. It has also been used to study human existence itself, most notably by Martin Heidegger and Jean-Paul Sartre. 1.4 Beware! Ultimately, science is just careful observation plus careful thinking. So we personality psychologists do the best we can with our research methods. That does leave us to consider the business of careful thinking, though, and there are a couple of particulars there to consider as well. First, we must always be on guard against ethnocentrism. Ethnocentrism is (for our purposes) the tendency we all have to see things from the perspective of our own culture. We are born into our culture, and most of us never truly leave it. We learn it so young and so thoroughly that it becomes "second nature." Sigmund Freud, for example, was born in 1856 in Moravia (part of what is now the Czech Republic). His culture – central European, German speaking, Victorian era, Jewish... – was quite different from our own (whatever that might be). One thing his culture taught was that sex was a very bad thing, an animal thing, a sinful thing. Masturbation was thought to lead to criminality, retardation, and mental illness. Women who were capable of orgasms were assumed to be nymphomaniacs, unlikely to make good wives and mothers, and possibly destined for prostitution. Freud is to be respected in that he was able to rise above his cultural attitudes about sex and suggest that sexuality – even female sexuality – was a natural (if animalistic) aspect of being human, and that repressing one's sexuality could lead to debilitating psychological disorders. On the other hand, he didn't quite see the possibility of a new western culture – our own – wherein sexuality was not only accepted as normal but as something we should all be actively engaged in at every opportunity. A second thing to be on guard against is egocentrism. Again, for our purposes, we are talking about the tendency to see our experiences, our lives, as being the standard for all people. Freud was very close to his mother. She was 21 when she had him, while his father was 40. She stayed home to raise him, while his father was working the usual 16 hour days of the time. Little Freud was a child genius who could talk about adult matters by the time he was five. He was, as his mother once put it, her "golden Siggy." C George Boeree, PhD: Personality Theory: A Biosocial Approach, © 2009. 9 1. Introduction These circumstances are unusual, even for his time and place. Yet, as he developed his theory, he took it for granted that the mother-son connection was at the center of psychology for one and all! That, of course, was a mistake: egocentrism. Last, we need to be on guard against dogmatism. A dogma is a set of ideas that the person who holds those ideas will not permit to be criticized. Do you have evidence against my beliefs? I don't want to hear them. Do you notice some logical flaws in my arguments? They are irrelevant. Dogmas are common in the worlds of religion and politics, but they have absolutely no place in science! Science should always be open to new evidence and criticism. Science isn't "Truth;" it is just a movement in the general direction of truth. When someone claims they have "Truth," science comes to a grinding halt. Well, sadly, Freud was guilty of dogmatism. He became so attached to his ideas that he refused to accept disagreement from his "disciples." (Notice the religious term here!) Some, like Jung and Adler, would eventually go on to develop their own theories. If only Freud had not been dogmatic, if only he had been open to new ideas and new evidence and allowed his theory to evolve openly, we might all be "Freudians" today – and "Freudian" would mean something quite different and much grander. C George Boeree, PhD: Personality Theory: A Biosocial Approach, © 2009. 10

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.